Kathmandu
Monday, October 20, 2025

Thinking Clearly in the Age of Confusion

October 20, 2025
7 MIN READ
A
A+
A-

In my previous opinion article in Nepal News, The Failure of the Educated Mind, I wrote about how our education system shapes educated minds who acquire a lot of knowledge but are not always encouraged to think critically. We learn to read, write, and memorize, but not to reason.

In this article, I want to take this reflection a step further and explore how understanding deductive, inductive, and abductive reasoning can help us think more clearly, not just in classrooms, but in everyday life and public discourse in Nepal.

Many people mistakenly think that deduction, induction, and abduction belong to the world of research methods, that deduction is for quantitative studies, induction is for qualitative ones, and abduction is a mix of the two. But that is not the case. These are not types of research; they are ways of reasoning. And understanding them is essential not only for researchers but for anyone who wishes to think clearly, argue responsibly, and make sense of the world.

Deductive reasoning begins with general principles or established facts and moves toward a specific conclusion. When the premises are true and the logic is valid, the conclusion must also be true. It gives us certainty. For example, if we accept that all humans need oxygen to survive and that Ram is human, we can confidently say that Ram needs oxygen to survive. That is deduction.

We often see similar patterns in Nepali politics too. When someone says, “All politicians are corrupt, and he is a politician, therefore he must be corrupt,” that sounds logical but relies on a faulty premise. Or when a party claims, “If you criticize the government, you must be against the nation,” it also uses deduction incorrectly, taking a narrow premise and stretching it to silence dissent. Sometimes we also hear, “Since it is called a youth revolution, the ruler should be young,” as if age alone defines leadership quality. These are examples of how deduction can sound logical while hiding weak or emotional assumptions. The problem arises when people use deduction carelessly, or when the premises themselves are faulty.

Take an absurd example: A stone doesn’t fly; humans don’t fly; therefore, humans are stones. The form of reasoning seems deductive, but the premises are meaningless. Yet, we often hear similar patterns in our public debates.

People take two unrelated facts and reach a grand conclusion as if logic alone could hold it together. That is not reasoning, it is rhetoric disguised as reasoning. Inductive reasoning, on the other hand, moves from specific observations to broader conclusions. It is about probability rather than certainty.

For example, if you notice that the sun rises every morning, you might reasonably conclude that it will rise again tomorrow. Induction allows us to make generalizations based on patterns we observe. However, inductive reasoning can also go wrong. For instance, if every time a political party has come to power, corruption has followed, one might conclude that this party too will be corrupt. The conclusion may sound convincing, but it is only probable, not guaranteed.

Similarly, when one leader fails, people often declare that democracy itself has failed, as if the failure of one person automatically discredits the whole system. These are classic examples of inductive overreach, generalizing from limited experience. We see this kind of thinking all the time in public discourse. “This leader lied once; therefore, all leaders lie.” Or “This foreign project failed once; therefore, all foreign aid is harmful.” Induction can reveal patterns, but it can also feed prejudice. Without critical reflection, inductive thinking can easily slide into stereotyping or cynicism. Abductive reasoning is often the most misunderstood.

Many believe it is simply a mixture of deduction and induction, a bit of theory here, a bit of observation there. But that is not what abduction is. Abduction is a distinct way of thinking. It is what we do when we encounter something surprising or confusing and search for the best possible explanation.

For example, imagine you come home and find the kitchen floor wet. Deductive reasoning might say, “If it rained and the window was open, the floor would be wet. It must have rained.” Inductive reasoning might say, “Every time the floor is wet, it has been raining. It probably rained again.” But abductive reasoning looks closer: “It didn’t rain, and the window is closed. Maybe the cat knocked over the water bowl.”

Abduction allows us to reason from surprise, to think creatively about what could explain what we see. In the context of Nepal, abductive reasoning is perhaps the skill we need most. Our society is filled with conspiracy theories, emotional reactions, and half-truths dressed as facts.

When someone says, “This happened because a foreign power planned it,” or “This community is behind that problem,” it often reflects an abductive impulse gone wrong, an attempt to explain without evidence, guided by suspicion rather than reason.

Abduction can also go astray when we jump to conclusions by association. For instance, if someone has taken a picture with a controversial figure, people quickly conclude they must be allies. Or if someone studied abroad or travels frequently, they are labeled as “foreign agents.” These are abductive leaps, attempts to explain by assumption, not by evidence. True abductive thinking looks for the best explanation, not just the most emotional or suspicious one.

Our public discourse today is full of reasoning errors. A common example is when someone says, “X has done bigger corruption and yet was not punished, so Y, who did a smaller offense, shouldn’t be in jail.” This is not reasoning; it is an escape from accountability and a way to justify wrongdoing by pointing at someone else.

Another familiar pattern is the claim that only those who live in Nepal are true nationalists, while those who go abroad for study or work are somehow less patriotic. This too is a form of faulty reasoning, it assumes moral superiority from geography, not from action or integrity. These kinds of fallacies persist because we were never taught to question the logic behind what we hear.

We were trained to accept information, not to interrogate it. Our classrooms emphasize memorization and obedience, not reasoning and reflection.

As a result, even the highly educated often confuse loudness for logic, confidence for truth, and emotion for evidence. If we brought the understanding of these modes of reasoning, deductive, inductive, and abductive, into our education system, it could transform the way we think.

Students could learn to identify weak arguments, to question premises, and to explore multiple explanations before settling on one. It would not only improve research skills but also nurture intellectual humility, the ability to say, “I may be wrong.” Interestingly, these ways of reasoning are not new to us.

Long before modern philosophy or research methods, Hindu and Buddhist thinkers had already explored these forms of inference. Ancient schools of logic in our region, such as Nyaya and Buddhist Pramana traditions, developed sophisticated systems of reasoning that closely resemble what we now call deduction, induction, and abduction.

So rather than borrowing something new, we might see this as returning to an old wisdom, the tradition of thinking clearly, questioning assumptions, and seeking truth through reflection and logic.

In today’s world, where we are bombarded with information, half-truths, and deliberate disinformation, these reasoning tools are not optional. They help us slow down before believing or sharing, to ask: Does this conclusion logically follow? What evidence supports it? Could there be another explanation?

Critical thinking is not just for academics; it is a civic responsibility. Without it, societies drift toward noise, confusion, and manipulation. With it, we build a culture of reason, where disagreement is not chaos but dialogue. So perhaps the problem of the educated mind in Nepal is not a lack of intelligence but a lack of reasoning.

We don’t need more information; we need better inference. Deduction, induction, and abduction are not mere research terms. They are the habits of a thoughtful society, the foundation of critical citizenship. If we can teach these habits early, not as abstract theories but as ways of living and thinking, we might begin to repair the broken link between education and wisdom. In the end, an educated mind is not one that knows everything, but one that knows how to think clearly.